Need a perfect paper? Place your first order and save 5% with this code:   SAVE5NOW

Open-Ended Question Essay on Pain Modulation

History of placebo analgesia

Many medical historians believe that the placebo effect has played a significant role in the development of medicine throughout time. Placebos didn’t become a staple of contemporary medicine until they were utilized as controls in clinical studies. In 1784, Louis XVI commissioned Benjamin Franklin and Antoine Lavoisier to examine Franz Mesmer’s claim that he had discovered “animal magnetism,” an alleged invisible force Mesmer thought held curative capabilities (Muller et al., 2016). Franklin and Lavoisier used placebos as controls in this study. Without notifying the patients, Franklin and Lavoisier exposed patients to “mesmerized” items or placebos (i.e., untreated objects). As a result of the researchers’ findings, they concluded that animal magnetism does not have any scientific foundation.

However, despite being widely employed in clinical studies, the placebo effect has been demoted to being regarded as just a nuisance variable to be controlled for since the development of the double-blind placebo-controlled study. This has been an unpleasant side consequence. There was little attention to placebo effects before the twentieth century. Beecher’s meta-analysis was perhaps the most important in this regard. A 35 percent average improvement in symptoms was found when Beecher merged the data from placebo groups from 15 separate trials on ailments like pain, seasickness, coughing, and anxiety, arguing for the potency and merit of the placebo effect (Annoni, 2020). There has been a lot of discussion about the placebo effect’s capacity to activate endogenous modulatory systems involved in self-healing procedures, despite Beecher’s technique being questioned later on.

Psychobiology and biochemistry of placebo and nocebo effects

Psychobiology and biochemistry of placebo effects; The placebo effect is typically explained using classical conditioning, a learning technique. Additionally, Pavlov (1927) found that a bell paired with the administration of morphine, which promotes restlessness in dogs, caused the dogs to become agitated when the bell was heard alone, giving the first indication that drug-like (placebo) effects may be conditioned. When it comes to the placebo effect, contextual cues (e.g., syringe, treatment room) are considered conditioned stimuli that may cause conditioned placebo effects when paired with an active therapy (e.g., morphine) (e.g., pain relief; the conditioned response) (Petrie & Rief, 2019). Social learning, also known as vicarious learning or modeling, may be used to produce placebo effects. It’s possible that the participants’ knowledge of placebo analgesia came from seeing the impact of the drug on others rather than through firsthand experience. Socially produced placebo analgesia is equivalent to conditioned responses and much more significant than vocally induced analgesia.

Psychobiology and biochemistry of nocebo effects; pain may be induced as a nocebo effect by anticipating its worsening by verbal disclosures or earlier exposure to unpleasant sensations (e.g., classical conditioning). Nocebo effects are equivalent in size to those generated by genuine experiences of increased somatosensory perception, discomfort, itching, and deterioration of motor function(Petrie & Rief, 2019). Because nocebo effects may also be generated by seeing other people suffer, social learning is essential in developing nocebo effects.

Negative expectations might have a counterintuitive effect on therapeutic effects. Bronchoconstrictors were characterized as bronchodilators by asthmatic patients, and the opposite was true when they were classified as bronchoconstriction by non-asthmatics. Furthermore, healthy patients who were given a stimulant, but were instead given a muscle relaxant drug, increased muscular tension (Požgain et al., 2014). In patients, nocebo effects may lead to psychological anguish, medication nonadherence, and the need for additional medications to alleviate the nocebo side effects.

How placebo and nocebo effects are formed

While “sugar” tablets are frequently linked with the placebo effect, many popular medications, such as analgesics and antidepressants, also include a placebo effect that contributes to their effectiveness. This is due to the belief that therapy would alleviate the patient’s discomfort (Palese et al., 2019). Positive treatment expectations (i.e., being informed the opioid will considerably lessen their pain) doubled the analgesic effect in a group of individuals administered a potent opioid and then exposed to painful stimuli.

On the other hand, the nocebo effect refers to a decrease in treatment effectiveness, an aggravation of symptoms, or the emergence of new side effects that do not occur due to the active therapy. 2 Since the treatment is seen to inflict damage, and this is why many are afraid of it (Klinger et al., 2017). The research found that when patients were informed that the opioid would make them more sensitive to pain after the first impact wore off, it entirely abolished the opioid’s analgesic benefit. The nocebo effect may be responsible for a large percentage of the side effects of drugs.

Nocebo-induced effects may be explained by the patient’s increased awareness or sensitivity to regular day-to-day symptoms, such as aches, pains, exhaustion, mood swings, and sensory alterations, due to their expectation that their therapy would have unfavorable consequences (Klinger et al., 2017). An undesirable impact is then linked to the treatment, and these symptoms are taken into account in the diagnosis (Petrie & Rief, 2019). Treatment-related side effects are changes in the course of an illness or its symptoms, which may be linked to the use of a new drug or treatment strategy.

Ways to communicate for reducing pain perception and nocebo hyperalgesia in daily clinical practice

The use of framing tactics may help reduce the occurrence of nocebo-related occurrences. It’s possible to provide the same information to reduce stress, worry, tension, and pain. In recent research, pregnant women who requested epidural analgesia were asked to disclose their health information and verbally propose ways to alleviate their pain. When a woman was told about the epidural procedure, she was told either a typical depiction of the pain or a soft one that included some excellent features. “You are going to feel a giant bee sting; this is the worst part of the treatment” and “We are going to give you a local anesthetic that will numb the region, and you will be comfortable throughout the process” were the two disclosures made by the authors when executing the epidural analgesic process.

In the same manner, we all agree that the first disclaimer stated above is a standard approach to explaining the effects of the process. In contrast, the second description discusses the procedure expecting the anesthetic medication’s benefit. A blinded evaluation of the patient’s pain experience followed. Nocebo group women in labor reported much more pain during the local anesthetic injection than those who had the treatment coupled with calm and pleasant remarks and were warned to anticipate it to hurt like a bee sting. According to this research, the manner presented information may significantly influence results, underlining the need for thorough disclosures. Information may be presented in a way that minimizes the nocebo’s impact while respecting patients’ rights to be informed. An empathetic communication compatible with professional ethics should be included in every clinician’s practice.

Reliving one’s emotional trauma throughout therapy is essential. The patient’s mental health may be assessed and treated through psychological treatment. Taking the drug while the patient is in a happy state of mind can also assist them in having better expectations for the future. When discussing the therapy with the patient, be careful not to use any negative language. The patient should be allowed to ask questions about the drug. The nocebo effect may be reduced by addressing patients’ worries, diminishing their worry, and increasing their optimistic expectations and minimum anxiety. With good information, patients may change their impressions of a medicine. Adverse nocebo effects may be reduced by switching from pessimism to optimism regarding medicine.

When working with clients regularly, it’s critical to prevent unfavorable results from being unintentionally linked to the environment. To help patients who’ve had a bad encounter with medicine, you must assist them in their efforts to reassess their unfavorable recollections of a particular drug. Changing their perception of the medicine and reducing nocebo hyperalgesia can help. Instead, if the patient has unfavorable recollections of a prescription, it will be critical to alter the surrounding circumstances to achieve therapeutic efficacy. It may be performed by switching to a generic version of a drug. It will lessen the nocebo effect by lowering patients’ unfavorable perceptions of a specific medicine. Drug wrapping or packaging may also be used to alleviate unwanted memories. As a result, it will lessen the patient’s unpleasant reaction to it.

Three critical ethical issues related to placebo and nocebo effects and four potential solutions

The present understanding of the placebo and nocebo effect gives us the information needed to begin an ethical dialogue. To do so, three fundamental ethical problems should be addressed. First is consequentialism. a concept of ethics in which the effects of one’s actions determine the morality of that action. The optimum outcomes of a specific action might vary based on the context and the value theory being considered. The second is informed consent. Ethics requires informed consent for medical treatment. To make well-informed choices regarding treatment, patients have the right to be informed and to ask questions. The patient-physician connection may be strengthened through effective communication. Lastly is autonomy, the right to control one’s destiny. It is within the purview of everyone who is morally capable of making decisions about their own lives.

First is the involvement of patients and the public in research planning. While some deceit will be included in the research, it will not hurt the people. A team of public members may participate in this stage to serve as witnesses. The second is authorized deception. According to this method, participants are informed they would be deprived of information that is not detrimental to them but critical to their participation in the study. If necessary, it may be given to them as soon as the data gathering is finished. The third is debriefing participants. Participants may be debriefed as early as practicable during the data collecting phase. Informing participants that deception was utilized because it was necessary to the exam but that the test was not damaging should be included. It will assist in resolving the ethical dilemma of individual agencies. Lastly is the participant-nominated informed consent. Nominated informed consent may be achieved by designating a trusted third party to receive and agree on their behalf to disclose personal information.

References

Webster, R. K., Weinman, J., & Rubin, G. J. (2018). Ethical issues surrounding the study of nocebo effects: Recommendations for deceptive research. British journal of health psychology, 23(4), 775. (Webster et al., 2018)

Petrie, K. J., & Rief, W. (2019). Psychobiological mechanisms of placebo and nocebo effects: pathways to improve treatments and reduce side effects. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 599-625.

Klinger, R., Blasini, M., Schmitz, J., & Colloca, L. (2017). Nocebo effects in clinical studies: hints for pain therapy. Pain reports, 2(2).

Palese, A., Rossettini, G., Colloca, L., & Testa, M. (2019). The impact of contextual factors on nursing outcomes and the role of placebo/nocebo effects: a discussion paper. Pain Reports4(3).

Požgain, I., Požgain, Z., & Degmečić, D. (2014). Placebo and nocebo effect: a mini-review. Psychiatria Danubina26(2), 0-107.

Annoni, M. (2020). Better than nothing: a historical account of placebos and placebo effects from modern to contemporary medicine. International Review of Neurobiology153, 3-26.

Müller, M., Kamping, S., Benrath, J., Skowronek, H., Schmitz, J., Klinger, R., & Flor, H. (2016). Treatment history and placebo responses to experimental and clinical pain in chronic pain patients. European Journal of Pain20(9), 1530-1541.

 

Don't have time to write this essay on your own?
Use our essay writing service and save your time. We guarantee high quality, on-time delivery and 100% confidentiality. All our papers are written from scratch according to your instructions and are plagiarism free.
Place an order

Cite This Work

To export a reference to this article please select a referencing style below:

APA
MLA
Harvard
Vancouver
Chicago
ASA
IEEE
AMA
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Copy to clipboard
Need a plagiarism free essay written by an educator?
Order it today

Popular Essay Topics